注册 找回密码
搜索
查看: 1032|回复: 1
打印 上一主题 下一主题

Nautre:大脑喜欢什么食物 肉类

[复制链接]
跳转到指定楼层
1#
发表于 2016-3-5 10:34:00 | 只看该作者 回帖奖励 |倒序浏览 |阅读模式
Nautre:大脑喜欢什么食物

2016-03-04 18:03



2016年3月4日 讯 /生物谷BIOON/ - - 人类在进化的过程中,除了食用植物外,更重要的是通过消费了其它动物的肉帮助自己进化出更高级的大脑。但是,就目前而言,我们的世界已经充满了各种各样的食物,那么对我们来说肉食还有那么大的作用吗?

先回顾一下人类的进化历史。大约6百万年前,人类的祖先从热带森林迁移到了热带草原生活。在那个时代,热带草原上有丰富的水果和蔬菜可供早期人类生存。但是,到了距今3百万年,一场巨大的气候变化导致了热带草原上食物资源缺乏。这场大灾难导致很多哺乳动物灭绝,也包括一大部分的灵长类。考古学家在埃塞俄比亚考古遗址发现了大约2百60万年前的动物化石。来自马德里康普顿斯大学的人类考古学家Manuel Dominguez-Rodrigo说:"在这些动物化石上存在着明显被屠宰和切割的痕迹。"

"在这场大灾难中仅存活下来两种灵长类,"Dominguez-Rodrigo说:"在这两种灵长类中,对植物和肉类都适应。它们的食物一方面来自植物,另一方面来自其他动物的肉。最后证明,灵长类进化出食肉能力使的他们进化出更大、更复杂大脑。" 所以说是食肉让我们的祖先进化成现在的我们!



我们的祖先是从动物肉类获取到铁、锌元素和维生素B12和脂肪酸等,为进化出这样的大脑提供了物质基础。肉类里面的铁元素超过任何蔬菜和水果,铁元素对胎儿的大脑发育中神经元的增长和分化非常重要。锌元素在海马区浓度非常高,对学习和记忆都有帮助。维生素B12可以保护神经鞘,像一些Omega-3脂肪酸(如DHA)可以辅助维持神经活性并调节大脑中的炎症反应等。

20世纪80年代,研究人员开始怀疑在一些贫困地区(墨西哥、肯尼亚和埃及)小孩由于缺乏肉类食物所造成了身材矮小、免疫力低下、学习和认知能力差等现象。

Neumann 带领的研究小组对肯尼亚12所学校6-14岁的儿童完成了一项实验,2年的实验结果发现在经常吃肉比吃玉米、豆类和蔬菜的学生有更多的肌肉含量,更少的健康问题,认知能力更强,数学和语言成绩都更优。

许多研究都已经证明了在缺乏的状态下肉类、蛋白质和维生素B对人具有重要的作用。来自澳大利亚国立大学的Diane Hosking做了一项研究,调查对象来自中等收入或高收入、认知健康65-90岁的人群。Hoskin发现测试结果志愿者的测试表现和他们在童年时期食肉的多少没有相关性。而现在肉的作用如何帮助大脑功能仍需要继续研究,另一方面需要更深入地了解大脑神经元的发育过程和认知记忆的机理。

对大众来说,现在最为关心的问题可能还是该多吃肉还是少吃肉。吃肉对认知健康的是否有影响,答案是肯定的。童年吃肉太少的话会延缓神经系统的发育和认知能力提升,而现代人如果吃过多的肉类,来自一些大规模生产的低质量肉类可能会造成其它健康问题。比如说心脏疾病、癌症的发生。(生物谷Bioon.com)

生物谷推荐英文原文报道:Brain food: Clever eating

http://www.nature.com/nature/jou ... rtId=ODgwMjU1NzcxS0



http://news.bioon.com/article/6679302.html  L
2#
发表于 2016-3-6 16:27:13 | 只看该作者

大脑喜欢什么食物:肉类

Nautre

大脑喜欢什么食物:肉类



顾汉现

优秀论文!

但是有数处翻译错误,故请(生物谷网站)订正(可请对照英文原文):

第二段中:
大约60亿年前  请订正为:大约6百万年前
到了距今30亿年  请订正为:到了距今3百万年
大约26亿年前  请订正为:大约2百60万年前

第七段中:
来自爱大利亚国立大学  请订正为:来自澳大利亚国立大学
志愿者的测试表现和特澳门在童年时期  请订正为:志愿者的测试表现和他们在童年时期

谢谢!
2016-3-6



顾汉现

英原文:
Around 6 million years ago,
some 3 million years ago,
almost 2.6 million years.
翻译错了。
故应订正为:
第二段中:
大约60亿年前 请订正为:大约6百万年前
到了距今30亿年 请订正为:到了距今3百万年
大约26亿年前 请订正为:大约2百60万年前



顾汉现

相当不错的论文!



顾汉现

英原文摘录:

Around 6 million years ago, primates started moving from tropical forests into the savannahs. Unlike today, these prehistoric expanses were humid and probably provided a year-round supply of fruit and vegetables. But then, some 3 million years ago, the climate changed and the savannahs — along with their plentiful food supply — dried up.

Many mammals, including some primates, went extinct, but others adapted. Archaeologists working at sites in modern Ethiopia have discovered animal remains that date back almost 2.6 million years. The telltale cut marks on their bones are almost certainly signs of butchery1, says Manuel Domínguez-Rodrigo, a palaeoanthropologist at Complutense University in Madrid.

2016-3-7



顾汉现

有些网站标题意译为:
大脑喜欢什么食物:肉类

2016-3-8



NATURE英文论文摘要:

Archive
Volume 531
Issue 7592
Outlook
Article
NATURE | OUTLOOK

Brain food: Clever eating

Sujata Gupta
Nature 531, S12–S13 (03 March 2016) doi:10.1038/531S12a
Published online 02 March 2016
Article tools
PDF
Citation
Reprints
Rights & permissions
Article metrics
Consumption of animals helped hominins to grow bigger brains. But in a world rich with food, how necessary is meat?

Subject terms: Brain Anthropology Nutrition Neuroscience

Jon Foster/National Geographic Creative/Bridgeman Images
Early humans who hunted animals for meat developed bigger brains than plant eaters.

Around 6 million years ago, primates started moving from tropical forests into the savannahs. Unlike today, these prehistoric expanses were humid and probably provided a year-round supply of fruit and vegetables. But then, some 3 million years ago, the climate changed and the savannahs — along with their plentiful food supply — dried up.

Many mammals, including some primates, went extinct, but others adapted. Archaeologists working at sites in modern Ethiopia have discovered animal remains that date back almost 2.6 million years. The telltale cut marks on their bones are almost certainly signs of butchery1, says Manuel Domínguez-Rodrigo, a palaeoanthropologist at Complutense University in Madrid.

Only two types of primate survived the climate catastrophe, says Domínguez-Rodrigo. There was a “plant-processing machine on the one hand and a meat-eating machine on the other hand”, he says. “The meat-eating machine evolved a bigger brain.”

The meat-eating machine became us.

To build and maintain a more complex brain, our ancestors used ingredients found primarily in meat, including iron, zinc, vitamin B12 and fatty acids. Although plants contain many of the same nutrients, they occur in lower quantities and often in a form that humans cannot readily use. For instance, red meat is rich in iron derived from haemoglobin, which is more easily absorbed than the non-haem form found in beans and leafy greens. Furthermore, compounds known as phytates bind to the iron in plants and block its availability to the body. As a result, meat is a much richer dietary source of iron than any plant food (see 'Meat efficiency'). “You would need to eat a massive amount of spinach to equal a steak,” says Christopher Golden, an ecologist and epidemiologist at Harvard University in Cambridge, Massachusetts.

The implications for cognitive health are huge. There is a clear, but underappreciated link between meat and the mind, says Charlotte Neumann, a paediatrician at the University of California, Los Angeles, who has studied meat eating in Africa and India for the past three decades. Deficiencies in the micronutrients found in meat have been linked with brain-related disorders, including low IQ, autism, depression and dementia. Iron is crucial for the growth and branching of neurons while in the womb; zinc is found in high concentrations in the hippocampus, a crucial region for learning and memory; vitamin B12 maintains the sheaths that protect nerves; and omega-3 fatty acids such as docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) help to keep neurons alive and to regulate inflammation.


Full size image (161 KB)
Meat for the poor
In the 1980s, researchers began to suspect that a lack of meat in some poor rural villages was contributing to a spectrum of childhood problems, including short stature, weakened immunity, social difficulties and poor school performance. When researchers from five universities studied the effects of chronic malnourishment in Mexico, Kenya and Egypt, they found that children who consumed the greatest amount of meat and dairy products scored highest on physical, cognitive and behavioural tests, particularly in Kenya2. But was the absence of meat really to blame? What the researchers needed was a controlled study.

So Neumann began a trial in Kenya3. Her team selected 12 schools with children aged 6 to 14, and gave some of the children midmorning snacks. Schools were divided into four groups: the control group was not given a snack, whereas the other three received variations on githeri, a traditional porridge that consists of maize (corn), beans and greens. One group received a basic version, the second received the basic githeri with a glass of milk, and the third had meat added; all githeri were balanced to contain the same amount of calories. The study continued for more than 2 years and spanned 2 cohorts, the first with 525 students and the second with 375. The students' physical health and classroom performance were measured every three or six months. Compared with the other groups, students in the meat group had greater muscle mass and fewer health problems, and even showed greater leadership in the playground. Cognitive performance was stronger, too: the meat group outperformed other groups in maths and language subjects4.

“Meat can be a worm, caterpillar or termite. It doesn't have to be butcher meat.”
Neumann was not surprised by the results. The typical diet in rural Kenya is subsistence-based and does not include many nutrients that help the brain to grow. The challenge now is to get people to consume more meat, which is widely regarded as too expensive. What people don't realize, Neumann says, is that to nourish the brain, pretty much any animal matter will do: “Meat can be a worm, caterpillar or termite. It doesn't have to be butcher meat.”

Meat for the rich
But how does meat fit into a richer diet? “A lot of the studies that have demonstrated the importance of meat, vitamin B, animal products and protein generally have been carried out in populations receiving very little nutrition,” says Diane Hosking, a healthy-ageing researcher at the Australian National University in Canberra.

To fill this gap, Hosking and her team asked 352 Australians aged between 65 and 90 years old — who were cognitively healthy and predominantly from a middle- or high-income background — to recall what sorts of food they ate growing up5, 6. For instance, how often did they eat items such as carrots, meat, fish or cake? The researchers then administered cognitive tests.

Hosking found no correlation between the volunteers' test performance and their consumption of meat as children. The results contradicted what Neumann and others have observed in developing countries. What's more, contrary to conventional wisdom, participants who consumed more fish during childhood and as adults were actually slower on measures of cognitive speed. (The fish might have contained neurocontaminants such as mercury, she says.)

There are several issues that affect these results, says Hosking. One is that people don't eat single foods, but patterns of foods, making it difficult to tease out the importance of an individual food type, such as meat. In the older Australians for instance, those who ate meat were also more likely to consume packaged desserts and snack foods.

Moreover, what the animal eats also matters. Livestock and poultry in Western nations are often raised in large facilities and fed diets that consist mainly of maize and soya, whereas animals from poor villages are typically farmed on a much smaller scale and forage for a greater variety of foods, which increases the nutrient content of their meat. Given these sorts of variations, Hosking says, “we have to be very cautious about making dietary recommendations ... for people who have access to large quantities of food.”

Meat in the brain
The micronutrients in meat have become an essential part of our diet over millennia. A few years ago, archaeologists in Tanzania unearthed fragments of a child's skull dating back 1.5 million years. Deformities on the bones suggested that the child had died from porotic hyperostosis, a condition thought to result from a deficiency in vitamin B12 — found exclusively in animal-derived foods. Humans started eating dairy products only in the past 5,000 years, meaning that the child had almost certainly died from a lack of meat7. So, by at least 1.5 million years ago, says Domínguez-Rodrigo, humans had become so adapted to eating meat that without it they would die.

Research is starting to provide some clues as to how meat helps the brain to function. Bradley Peterson, director of the Institute for the Developing Mind at Children's Hospital Los Angeles in California, has investigated why low iron levels in children are correlated with lower IQ and poor concentration8. Using magnetic resonance imaging, Peterson and his colleagues mapped out what happened in the brains of newborn infants of 40 adolescent mothers — a group known to be at high risk for iron deficiency. Although most of the women reported taking prenatal vitamins with iron, 58% had iron levels below normal and 14% met the criteria for mild anaemia.

As the brain develops, says Peterson, neurons become increasingly complex, forming branch-like dendrites covered with spines — much like a growing tree. The brain images that his team took showed a correlation between neuron complexity in an infant and the amount of iron in the mother's diet. “The higher the iron intake throughout pregnancy, the more mature or the more complex grey matter was at the time of birth,” says Peterson, who is continuing to track the mothers and babies to see how those variations play out.

Beyond simple measures of micronutrient intake, individual requirements are also influenced by a person's genetics. So far, much of the research has focused on how people process omega-3 fatty acids, chiefly DHA and eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA), which are crucial for human cognitive health.

Omega-3 fatty acids are found primarily in oily, wild fish, such as salmon and tuna, but pasture-raised animals are also a good source. (Animals fed only soya or maize have fewer omega-3s.) In 2012, researchers discovered that most African populations, but not European populations, carried a variant of the FADS gene that made them more efficient at converting omega-3s in plants into a usable form, meaning that they required less from animal sources9. Conversely, a 2014 paper reported that people carrying a variant of the APOE gene (11–17% of US individuals of European descent) that confers a greater risk of developing late-onset Alzheimer's disease, derived little benefit from eating fatty fish10. “One size does not fit all around nutritional recommendations,” says Hosking. Put another way, the nutrients found in meat are important for health and cognition, but only up to a point. “Meat packs a lot of minerals and vitamins in just a small amount of food,” says Domínguez-Rodrigo. “Eating meat is like eating a power bar.”

So the key question becomes how much meat should a cognitive-health-conscious person eat. Too little can delay development and cognition. But too much, particularly if it is low quality and mass produced, is associated with other health concerns, such as heart disease and cancer, along with memory problems later in life. A person's life stage matters: pregnant women need more iron, as do babies and children. Genetics also play a part, but we don't yet know all the particulars. All these caveats make for a murky takeaway.

References
References• Author information• Related links
Domínguez-Rodrigo, M., Rayne Pickering, T., Semaw, S. & Rogers, M. J. J. Hum. Evol. 48, 109–121 (2005).
PubMedArticle
Show context
Neumann, C., Bwibo, N. O. & Sigman, M. Final Report Phase II: Functional Implications of Malnutrition, Kenya Project. Nutrition CRSP. (University of California, Los Angeles, 1992).
Show context
Neumann, C. G., Murphy, S. P., Gewa, C., Grillenberger, M. & Bwibo, N. O. J. Nutr. 137, 1119–1123 (2007).
CASPubMed
Show context
Hulett, J. L. et al. Br. J. Nutr. 111, 875–886 (2014).
CASPubMedArticle
Show context
Hosking, D. E., Nettelbeck, T., Wilson, C. & Danthiir, V. Br. J. Nutr. 112, 228–237 (2014).
CASPubMedArticle
Show context
Hosking, D. & Danthiir, V. Br. J. Nutr. 110, 2069–2083 (2013).
CASPubMedArticle
Show context
Domínguez-Rodrigo, M. et al. PLoS ONE 7, e46414 (2012).
CASPubMedArticle
Show context
Monk, C. et al. Pediatric Res. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/pr.2015.248 (2015).
Show context
Mathias, R. A. et al. PLoS ONE 7, e44926 (2012).
CASPubMedArticle
Show context
Chouinard-Watkins, R. & Plourde, M. Nutrients 6, 4452–4471 (2014).
CASPubMedArticle
Show context

http://www.nature.com/nature/jou ... rtId=ODgwMjU1NzcxS0

您需要登录后才可以回帖 登录 | 注册

本版积分规则

关于都市网 | 服务条款 | 开放平台 | 广告服务 | 商务洽谈 | 都市网招聘 | 都市网公益 | 客服中心 | 网站导航 | 版权所有

手机版|小黑屋|Comsenz Inc.  

© 2001-2013 源码论坛 Inc.    Powered by Weekend Design Discuz! X3.2

GMT+8, 2024-5-9 06:35 , Processed in 0.076586 second(s), 21 queries .

快速回复 返回顶部 返回列表